Generality
-
some generalisations are causal, some are arrived at by analogy and some
arguments which are technically analogical (weather forecasts) methodologically
resemble causal and generalising arguments.
-
generalisations claim for all something believed known about some. In each generalisation there is a conclusion
and a basis for it. The conclusions are
all general statements, not themselves reasoning, but rather the results of
reasoning.
General
statements have been characterised by their outward form, by their containing
certain terms in certain slots.
Generalisations
are about “all”, “no” or the
equivalent. Generalisations can be
either true or false. Generalisations
can be factual or definitional.
Hard
generalisations are characterised by language such
as “each”, “every”, “not one single...”
etc. and can be falsified by one or more counter instances.
When
a general statement is improperly objected to on the basis of a
so-called “exception” which it never
was intended to cover, we have the fallacy known as secundum quid or “from
a qualified statement to an unqualified one”
Moral
behaviour is introduced together with moral language and moral
principles.
Moral
principles are integral with the whole social fabric. When moral principles get separated from that
fabric, they get reduced to formulas.
Formulas
fail as a basis for moral action unless they are accompanied by a deeper
understanding.
Scientific
laws get evaluated by their range of applicability, by their felicity with
other theory, and by the accuracy of the deductions which can be made based on
them.
Generalisations
distribute - they apply
distributively (collective statements)
It
is easy to slide into thinking that what holds for the parts of the class must
hold for the class as a whole, and vice versa.
To argue from a characteristic of
the parts of a group to a characteristic of the whole is to commit the fallacy
of composition.
To
argue from a characteristic of a whole to a characteristic of the parts commits
the fallacy of division.
In
composition the term equivocated upon appears in the premises in a distributive
sense and in the conclusion in a collective sense.
In
division a term appearing collectively in the premises appears distributively
in the conclusion.
Much
good reasoning does proceed from parts to whole, and vice versa.
Not
many statistical generalisations are distributive. A statistical generalisation applies a
percentage or proportion to a class.
What
counts is to generalise intelligently and to be articulate in assessing the
generalising of others.
Simple
and statistical projections frequently provide exact and powerful tools for
getting the job done.
The
art of induction, of making intelligent projections, is called
sampling.
The
goal of sampling is to obtain a representative sample, one which contains
proportionally all relevant characteristics of the class projected to.
An
unrepresentative sample is said to be biased.
The
class projected to and sampled from is called the universe or population.
-
sometimes a representative sample can be obtained by making a random or
probability sample, one in which each member of the population has an equal
chance of being selected.
“Random”
is relative. Random with respect to what
population?
Changes
can occur in the population between the sampling time and the time of the event
projected to.
Sampling
can be biased itself as in sampling without replacement.
Failure
to distinguish between sampling with and sampling without replacement may
account for the psychological trick called the “gamblers” or “Monte Carlo”
fallacy, in which the victim imagines that each successive loss increases the
chances of a win on the next round. A
vague analogy brought over from the gamblers previous experience in games where
non-replacement is practiced.
A
stratified random sample can be taken wherever variations in the parent
group are familiar and known to effect the result.
Systematic
or interval sampling selects every nth item beginning
at a randomly selected starting point.
Cluster
sampling is taking the sample in clusters as the clusters
occur in the parent group.
Quota
sampling, an investigator selects so many instances in each
stratum of a stratified sample, not at random, but as they are found.
Besides
varying the instances, the responsible generaliser should get enough instances.
The
topic of proper sample size becomes technical very quickly.
Proper
sample size depends on the desired precision of the results, on the number of
instances in the sample, on the variation within the sample, and on the size of
the population. (perhaps surprising population size is usually the least
important factor in the list).
Two
important elements in determining what is the appropriate sample size are (1)
the margin of error which can be tolerated, and (2) the desired degree of
confidence that the result will be within that margin.
The
degree of precision in any sample is called its sampling error, an index
of the probability of a sample of that
size falling within a certain percentage range (it is theoretical margin of
error).
All
things being equal, the precision of a sample increases with the square root of
the sample size.
Polls
and surveys are best seen as morally neutral - tools which can be used for good
ends or for bad.
One
ought not to take polls at face value.
Any poll worth considering ought to display its methodology.
Opinion
survey reports ought to state at least the following (1) the sponsor’s name and
surveyor’s name, (2) the sample size (and sampling error), (3) the date of
contact, (4) exactly what population is being sampled and the method of
contact, (6) the exact questions asked.
Responsible
surveys consisting of people’s responses to questions must be designed in order
to eliminate the charge that the questions themselves colour the results. Questions undergo scrutiny by experienced
critics in order to uncover poor wording.
Then they are tested on samples of respondents. The questions are asked, then followed up
with questioning designed to bring out respondents comprehension and true
feelings. Alternative formulations may
be given to split samples in order to detect differences between formulations.
Questions
must be easy to understand.
Good
questions should be short and cover one subject.
Questions
should be divided, one question for goal, the other for the means.
Questions
should be worded so as to avoid colouring the responses eg. “Are you a racist?”
People
are inclined to say what they think the pollster wants to hear, and not what in
fact they think.
People
also tend to project what they imagine to be a favourable self-image.
No comments:
Post a Comment